Reading & Literacy

Reading and the Mind: A Talk With Dan Willingham

By Liana Loewus — May 30, 2017 6 min read
Daniel Willingham, a psychology professor at the University of Virginia, takes a break during an interview at the university.
  • Save to favorites
  • Print

Daniel Willingham has long been interested in how learning and memory work.

But about 15 years ago, the University of Virginia professor of psychology decided to move beyond the study of cognition and do something few others in his field had done: focus on what the research means for classrooms.

His goal these days is to help K-12 teachers understand why students learn the way they do.

“My experience is teachers are kind of tired of hearing, ‘If you do X, then Y will happen,’ ” Willingham, who is married to a Montessori teacher and credits her with guiding much of his research, said. “They’re just supposed to believe it. It’s pretty patronizing. ... It wouldn’t satisfy me. It’s much more persuasive if you understand how it works.”

Willingham has written several successful books that bridge the gap between cognitive psychology and education, including Why Don’t Students Like School? and Raising Kids Who Read.

His most recent book, The Reading Mind, published last month, is a deep dive into the many processes happening as people translate black marks on a page into meaning. It’s an ambitious undertaking, covering everything from why sound-based-versus picture-based-coding systems were created to how reading on digital devices affects comprehension.

Rather than prescribing how to teach, the book “is meant to leave the teacher with a useful cartoon model of what’s happening in the mind when a skilled reader reads,” Willingham explained.

Below are some highlights distilled from both an interview with Education Week and Willingham’s new book.

Using Sound and Sight to Decode

The purpose of writing-and, by extension, reading-is to communicate thought across time and space, Willingham explains in his book. Writing with pictures or symbols requires too much memorization, so instead, sound-based decoding systems were developed, in which the sounds that make up spoken language are written down.

Sound-based decoding requires three things: 1) the ability to distinguish letters from each other (to see the difference between “b” and “p,” for instance); 2) the ability to tell sounds from one another (to hear the difference between “b” and “p”); and 3) the knowledge about which sounds go with which letters or letter pairs.

Watch: Daniel Willingham Talks Reading and the Mind

“It’s the second of those that, if it’s going to be a stumbling block, that’s probably what’s going to be hardest for kids,” Willingham said. “None of it is supereasy. I mean the easiest is letters probably.”

But when kids really have difficulty in learning decoding, it’s likely because they struggle with phonemic awareness, or hearing the differences between sounds.

Experienced readers don’t have to sound words out-instead, they remember what words look like. With sight-based reading, “what really counts is reading experience,” he said. “Eventually, most typically developing readers are going to develop this type of visual expertise where they become fluent” in recognizing words.

Understanding What You Read

A very simple way of looking at reading comprehension is that it’s not that different from understanding speech.

“Once you’ve got a fluent decoder, everything else that’s happening in reading is basically the same thing that’s happening when you’re listening to somebody talk,” he said. “We know that’s not fully right, but to a first degree of approximation, it’s pretty close.”

On a deeper level, reading comprehension requires an understanding of individual words, what those words mean when they are put together to form a sentence, and how sentences connect to each other.

“Knitting sentences together is a very important part of comprehension, and it’s the part that students usually have difficulty with or fail to do altogether,” Willingham said.

Having background knowledge is key to understanding how sentences fit together, Willingham asserts. He uses this example in the book: “The morning precipitation had left sidewalks icy. Kayla told her children to be careful.” To make the connection there, the reader has to know, among other things, that people walk on sidewalks, that ice is slippery, that people can fall and get hurt when they walk on slippery things, and that parents don’t want their children to get hurt.

(A University of Virginia professor emeritus, E.D. Hirsch Jr., spent his career arguing that background knowledge is critical for reading comprehension. Hirsch and Willingham are pals, it turns out, and agree on the point, though Willingham notes that he believes knowing grammar and other “content-free abstract rules” are important for comprehension as well.)

When Reading Goes Digital

Overall, the research shows that reading on a screen can hurt comprehension a bit, Willingham explained.

“If I had to guess, that will probably be gone in 10 years as we get better and better at figuring out why,” he said. The data differ slightly depending on the kind of digital reading being done.

When it comes to reading a novel on an e-reader versus on paper, there’s “not a whole lot of difference between the two formats,” he said. “There’s probably a small hit to reading comprehension on the screen.”

But most often, people use Kindles and other e-readers for pleasure reading, so that kind of small hit is OK.

For digital textbooks, there seems to be a slightly larger negative impact on comprehension, Willingham said. However, some studies have shown comprehension is about the same; it just takes longer to read a digital textbook than a paper one.

That all makes sense, he said, because in a digital textbook, “the content is hard-it’s complicated stuff.”

And while digital technology seemed to have a lot of promise for improving prereading interventions, say, by using an iPad app to practice letter sounds, the data there are “really all over the place,” Willingham said.

That is, some applications help improve reading skills and some don’t.

Software developers are, for the most part, “just using intuition for how to do this, and the design choices they’re making end up having an impact,” he said. Animations and graphics may be illuminating or distracting, depending on how they’re used.

In all, using technology to improve reading “may be more complicated than we thought.”

Reading Motivation

In his new book, Willingham also rehashes some of Raising Kids Who Read, which looks at fostering a love of reading. Rewards aren’t the best option for getting students to read more, he writes. “A reward definitely makes it more likely you’ll do something,” he said. “My concern is what happens when the reward ends.”

Studies show that rewards can backfire; people who are rewarded for doing a task may think the task was less enjoyable afterward than those who were not rewarded for doing the same task. That’s because they attribute their participation to the reward alone.

Offering a logical appeal for why students should read-such as telling them that it will broaden their mind or help them in school-isn’t that effective, either. Attitudes about reading, instead, tend to be based on emotion.

“The analogy to exercise is compelling,” Willingham said. “My problem is not that I don’t understand the health benefits of exercise, it’s that I just don’t like it.”

To engender positive reading attitudes, students need to have positive reading experiences, Willingham writes. They need to see themselves as readers. And they need to have books that they enjoy readily available.

Related Tags:

A version of this article appeared in the May 30, 2017 edition of Education Week as Reading and the Mind: An Author Q&A

Events

This content is provided by our sponsor. It is not written by and does not necessarily reflect the views of Education Week's editorial staff.
Sponsor
Artificial Intelligence Webinar
Teaching Students to Use Artificial Intelligence Ethically
Ready to embrace AI in your classroom? Join our master class to learn how to use AI as a tool for learning, not a replacement.
Content provided by Solution Tree
This content is provided by our sponsor. It is not written by and does not necessarily reflect the views of Education Week's editorial staff.
Sponsor
Teaching Webinar
Empowering Students Using Computational Thinking Skills
Empower your students with computational thinking. Learn how to integrate these skills into your teaching and boost student engagement.
Content provided by Project Lead The Way
This content is provided by our sponsor. It is not written by and does not necessarily reflect the views of Education Week's editorial staff.
Sponsor
IT Infrastructure & Management Webinar
The Reality of Change: How Embracing and Planning for Change Can Shape Your Edtech Strategy
Promethean edtech experts delve into the reality of tech change and explore how embracing and planning for it can be your most powerful strategy for maximizing ROI.
Content provided by Promethean

EdWeek Top School Jobs

Teacher Jobs
Search over ten thousand teaching jobs nationwide — elementary, middle, high school and more.
View Jobs
Principal Jobs
Find hundreds of jobs for principals, assistant principals, and other school leadership roles.
View Jobs
Administrator Jobs
Over a thousand district-level jobs: superintendents, directors, more.
View Jobs
Support Staff Jobs
Search thousands of jobs, from paraprofessionals to counselors and more.
View Jobs

Read Next

Reading & Literacy Can a New Reading Lawsuit Finally Answer: What Is Research-Based Curriculum, Anyway?
The reading series were deceptively marketed as backed by research, despite omitting key instructional elements, the lawsuit claims.
7 min read
An elementary student reads on his own in class.
An elementary student reads on his own in class.
Allison Shelley for All4Ed
Reading & Literacy What the Research Says What’s in the ‘Secret Sauce’ That Made This Virtual Reading Tutoring Work?
High attendance, well-trained tutors, and trusting relationships helped close learning gaps.
4 min read
Teaching and tutoring online to a young child at home.
Getty/E+
Reading & Literacy 4 Things to Know About the Literacy Lawsuit Targeting Lucy Calkins and Fountas & Pinnell
A novel lawsuit could open a new front in the reading wars. Here's what you need to know.
6 min read
Two students in a combined second- and third-grade class read together.
Allison Shelley for All4Ed
Reading & Literacy Parents Sue Lucy Calkins, Fountas and Pinnell, and Others Over Reading Curricula
A first-of-its-kind lawsuit targets creators of whole-language reading curricula.
6 min read
Volunteer teacher reading to a class of preschool kids, preschool age; school children; students.
iStock/Getty